Received: 20 September 2018 Revised: 13 March 2019 Accepted: 28 March 2019 DOI: 10.1111/ijpo.12534 WILEY pediatricobesity ORIGINAL RESEARCH The role of weight stigma in parental weight talk Ellen V. Pudney1,2 | Mary S. Himmelstein2 1 Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, University of Connecticut, Mansfield, Connecticut Correspondence Ellen Pudney, MS, RD, Rudd Center for Food Policy and Obesity University of Connecticut, One Constitution Plaza, Suite 600, Hartford 06103, CT. Email: ellen.pudney@gmail.com Rebecca M. Puhl1,2 Summary Background: 2 Rudd Center for Food Policy and Obesity, University of Connecticut, Hartford, Connecticut | Many parents engage in “weight talk” with their child, such as encour- aging their child to lose weight or making comments about their own weight or others' weight. What parents say to their children about weight can affect children's well‐being, yet little is known about parental characteristics that may be at play regarding this common practice. Objectives: This study examined the role of weight stigma in parental weight talk, and whether internalized weight bias mediates the relationship between parents' experiences of weight stigma and weight‐based conversations and comments. Methods: A sample of 453 parents (Mage = 35.07) with children 2 to 17 years old (Mage = 8.76) completed surveys measuring parental experiences with weight stigma, weight bias internalization, and frequency of different types of parental weight talk. Mediations were tested via the PROCESS macro. Results: Independent of controls (parent BMI, child BMI percentile, parent and child sex, parent and child age, race, income, and education), experienced weight stigma was indirectly associated with greater frequency of weight‐based conversations (β = .08) and comments about oneself (β = .08) and others (β = .04) via parents' internalized weight bias. Conclusions: These findings offer novel insights for understanding parental weight talk and can inform pediatric providers working with families on weight‐related issues. K E Y W OR D S communication, obesity, parent, weight bias, weight stigma, weight talk 1 | I N T RO D U CT I O N without reference to weight (child‐centred health conversations), impersonal/indirect weight comments about oneself or others (paren- Body weight is an emotionally charged issue for many children and 1 tal weight comments), and weight criticism/teasing of their children.5 adolescents, especially during periods of physical growth when soci- Data from a national sample of adolescents in the United States etal values of physical appearance become more salient. What parents suggest that approximately one‐third of parents engage in conversa- say to their children about body weight can play an important role in tions about weight with their child.6 Weight conversations are most children's body esteem and physical well‐being.2-4 “Parental weight common amongst parent/child dyads of the same sex, of adolescents talk” is a general term that has been used to describe weight‐related categorized as overweight, and of adolescents of Hispanic/Latino communication by parents with their children, such as parental and Asian/Hmong descent.6 Evidence further suggests that parental encouragement of their child to lose or maintain weight (child‐centred weight comments may also occur frequently, especially from mothers; weight conversations), encouragement to exercise/eat healthy one study found that 27% of mothers reported “often or very often” Pediatric Obesity. 2019;e12534. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12534 wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ijpo © 2019 World Obesity Federation 1 of 11 --~-w1 LEY~•@•t®iiti•]•r41 i 2 of 11 PUDNEY ET AL. M f---------------------------- commenting on their own weight, while 13% of mothers reported literature documenting weight stigma as a prevalent problem with often commenting on others' weight.3 Weight‐focused conversations associated health consequences. Approximately 40% of adults report and comments from parents can occur in families regardless of the experiencing weight stigma, such as weight‐based teasing, unfair weight status of the child, but children and adolescents categorized treatment, or discrimination.14,15 These experiences are associated as overweight or with obesity tend to report more negative emotional with maladaptive eating behaviours, decreased physical activity, reactions in response to words parents use to describe their weight, depression, low self‐esteem, and body dissatisfaction.16,17 Further- compared with those classified at a normal weight.7 Children and more, weight stigma can also be internalized, which involves apply- adolescents with higher body weight also have increased vulnerability ing negative weight‐based stereotypes to themselves and blaming to stigma, bullying, and victimization because of their weight,8,9 themselves for their own weight status—a construct known as not just at school from peers,10 but also at home from family weight bias internalization.18-21 Thus, while experienced weight members.11 Thus, parental comments about weight could potentially stigma refers to weight‐based prejudice, stereotyping, and unfair exacerbate negative implications for the emotional well‐being of these treatment that people confront in extrinsic situations (such as being children who may already feel stigmatized and distressed because of stereotyped or treated unfairly in the workplace or by peers), weight their weight. bias internalization involves directing stigma inwardly on to oneself; Furthermore, weight conversations and comments have important an intrinsic process of self‐stigma in that people apply negative implications for both physical and mental health. Previous work dem- weight‐based stereotypes to themselves.18,21 Although these two onstrates child‐centred weight conversations, such as encouraging a constructs are highly correlated, weight bias internalization is child to change their behaviours for the purpose of managing their uniquely associated with poor emotional and physical health out- weight, are associated with body dissatisfaction amongst college‐aged comes,22,23 the effects of that can occur independent of experienced women, particularly when the encouragement comes from the girl's weight stigma.24-26 In some cases, internalizing weight bias may have 12 Amongst adolescent girls, both maternal and paternal more negative ramifications for health than experienced stigma weight conversations are associated with the use of unhealthy weight alone.24-26 For example, recent research has found that weight bias control behaviours (eg, skipping meals, vomiting) and binge eating internalization partially mediates the effect of experienced weight mother. 2 when the encouragement comes from the mother. Greater frequency stigma on exercise behaviour,26 and that feeling concerned about of mothers commenting on their own weight or the weight of others is stigma mediates the relationship between body mass index (BMI) associated with feelings of low self‐worth and depressive symptoms and self‐reported health, even when controlling for perceived weight amongst their daughters.3 Frequency of parental comments about discrimination.27 their own weight is associated with adolescent use of extreme weight Given the prevalence and consequences of experienced weight control behaviours.2 In contrast, parental conversations with children stigma, internalized weight bias in adults, and recent qualitative evi- that focus on healthy eating or being physically active, without talking dence suggesting that parental experiences of weight‐based stigma about body weight, have no evidence for associations with negative might influence child‐centred weight conversations,13 examining the health ramifications and may instead help prevent disordered eating relationships between experienced weight stigma, internalized weight behaviours.4,5 bias, and parental weight talk is warranted. It may be that parents who Although this emerging evidence suggests that parental weight talk experience weight stigma, but do not internalize these experiences, is prevalent and that weight‐focused comments and conversations avoid talking about weight with their child because of heightened con- from parents are potentially harmful for children's mental and physical cerns about the effects of weight talk on their child's well‐being. health, little is known about why some parents engage in this practice Alternatively, parents who experience weight stigma, and internalize more than others. In one qualitative study, parental engagement in these experiences, might choose to engage in weight talk with their weight‐focused conversations or teasing stemmed from several moti- child to encourage their child to lose weight in order to avoid being vations, including a health professional's opinion about their child's the target of weight‐based teasing. The direction of these relation- health, the parent's own concern for their child's health, and parental ships is unclear and has not been studied, but identifying whether, attempts to protect their child from being teased about their weight and to what extent, weight stigma plays a role in parental weight talk by others.13 Parents who avoided weight conversations/teasing with could offer important insights about strategies to improve parent‐child their children emphasized their own negative past experiences with communication on this issue. To begin to address these research gaps, weight conversations/teasing (and not wanting to repeat these expe- the present exploratory study had the following aims: (a) examine the riences for their child) and wanting their child to be respectful of relationship between parental experiences of weight stigma and the others' body sizes.13 This qualitative evidence highlights a potential frequency of their engagement in different forms of parental weight role of parental weight‐based stigma in influencing child‐centred talk, (b) examine the relationship between weight bias internalization weight conversations and the need for further research to examine amongst parents and the frequency of their engagement in different the relationship between parental experiences of weight stigma and forms of parental weight talk, and (c) assess the extent to that weight weight talk. bias internalization mediates the relationship between parents' experi- Weight stigma has been neglected in research on parental weight talk with children, which is surprising given the substantial enced weight stigma and the frequency of engagement in different forms of parental weight talk. PUDNEY ET AL. ---------------------------w• 2 TABLE 1 METHODS | LEY~•W•®Ht+t·)·t47 'rat- - '~-3 of 11 Sample characteristics Range M SD 35.07 8.19 Parent age (years) N 453 Parent BMI 452 15 64 27.21 6.88 explore parental communication and comments around weight. Of the Child age (years) 453 2 17 8.76 4.98 597 participants who entered the survey, 37 did not meet the eligibil- Child BMI percentile 0 100 65.64 33.95 2.1 | Participants A total sample of 597 adults (age 18 years or older) were surveyed to ity criteria of having at least one child between the ages of 2 and 451 20 N % 17 years living with them. Participants were also excluded for missing Parent sex demographic or anthropometric data (eg, sex, height, and weight; Male 184 40.6 Female 269 59.4 Male 242 53.4 Female 211 46.6 352 77.7 n = 35), skipping greater than 75% of the questions (n = 32), being pregnant (n = 1), or entering a biologically impossible height or weight for themselves or their child (n = 39), resulting in a final sample of 453 participants. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. Study participants consisted of 184 men and 269 women Child sex Race/ethnicity (41% and 59%, respectively). The mean age of parents was 35.07 years White, non‐Hispanic, non‐Latino (SD = 8.19, range 20‐65), while the mean age of their children was Black or African American 37 8.2 8.76 years (SD = 4.98, range 2‐17). Seventy‐eight percent of the sam- Asian or Pacific Islander 28 6.2 23 5.1 or Pacific Islander, 5% Latino/Hispanic, and 3% who indicated “Other” Latino, Hispanic, or Mexican‐American racial categories. The mean BMI of parents was 27.21 (SD = 6.88). Other 13 2.9 64 14.1 $25 000‐$49 999 140 30.9 $50 000‐$74 499 112 24.7 $75 000‐$99 999 70 15.5 $100 000‐$124 999 31 6.8 $125 000 or more 36 7.9 ple was White, followed by 8% Black or African American, 6% Asian Using guidelines of the Centers for Disease Control, 31% of parents were classified as overweight (almost identical to the US average of 31.8%) and 25% were classified as having obesity (lower than the US average of 39.8%).28 Using BMI percentile guidelines from the Centers for Disease Control, 15% of the reported children were classified as overweight and 27% were classified as having obesity (higher than US rates of obesity of 18.5% for children ages 2‐19 years).28 2.2 | Household income (US dollars, annually) Under $25 000 Education Procedures High school/GED, or less Data were collected from March to April 2016 via an online survey 29 that was advertised on Mechanical Turk (MTurk). The survey was advertised as a “Parent Survey on Communication about Weight.” Participants were compensated $1.50, which is a rate typical of other 58 12.8 Some college or technical/vocation degree 203 34.6 College graduate or higher 238 52.6 Parent BMI category MTurk studies of similar length.30 In addition to being a parent of a Underweight 12 2.6 child between the ages of 2 and 17 years living at home, parameters Normal weight 189 41.7 were restricted to participants living in the United States. Participants Overweight 138 30.5 Obese 113 24.9 40 8.8 provided informed consent and participation was anonymous and voluntary. All procedures were approved by the University of Connecticut institutional review board. Child BMI percentile category Underweight Normal weight Overweight 2.3 | Measures Participants completed self‐report measures to assess demographic characteristics, anthropometrics for both parent and child, parental weight talk (conversations and comments), experienced weight stigma, and internalization of weight bias. These measures are described below. 65 Obese 218 48.1 69 15.2 124 27.4 Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; GED, General Educational Development. --~-w1 LEY~•@•t®iiti•]•r41 i 4 of 11 2.3.1 PUDNEY ET AL. M f---------------------------- | these items,3 the “very often” response category was combined with Demographics “often,” resulting in a score ranging from 1 to 4 for both questions. Participants answered questions regarding their age, sex, race, education, household income, and their child's sex and date of birth, from 2.3.5 | Experienced weight stigma which we calculated the child's age. Experienced weight stigma was measured with two (yes/no) questions 2.3.2 | in that participants answered whether they had ever been teased or Anthropometrics treated unfairly because of their weight.34 Participants were coded Participants reported height and weight for themselves and their child. Parental BMI was calculated from self‐reported height and weight. While continuous BMI was used in regression analyses, BMI status was stratified into weight categories in accordance to guidelines from 31 the Centers for Disease Control as having experienced weight stigma if they answered “yes” to either of these questions. 2.3.6 | Weight bias internalization (see Table 1). Child BMI percentiles were calculated using the children's BMI group calculator from the Centers for Disease Control, which accounts for age and sex.31 Internalized weight bias was assessed using the modified weight bias internalization scale (WBIS‐M), which measures the extent to which people apply negative weight‐based stereotypes to themselves and blame themselves for their weight status.19-21 The 10‐item version 2.3.3 | Child‐centred weight and health conversations of the WBIS‐M was used, which is appropriate for individuals of diverse body weight categories and aligns with recent research recommending that the first item be dropped from the original Child‐centred weight and health conversations were measured using a 11‐item scale.20 Participants were asked to indicate their level of six‐item scale.6,32 Two out of the six questions assessed frequency of agreement with statements such as “I don't feel that I deserve to have child‐centred health conversations by asking parents how often in the a really fulfilling social life because of my weight.” Participants past year they had a conversation with their child(ren) about (a) responded using a seven‐point Likert scale ranging from “strongly healthy eating habits and (b) being physically active. The remaining disagree” to “strongly agree,” where higher scores indicate greater four questions assessed frequency of child‐centred weight conversa- weight bias internalization. Reponses to the 10 questions were aver- tions by asking parents how often in the past year they had a conver- aged resulting in an overall score from 1 to 7 (Cronbach's α = .95). sation with their child about his/her weight or size and told their child that he/she weighs too much, should eat differently, or exercise in order to lose weight or to keep from gaining weight. Participants responded using a five‐point Likert scale ranging from “never or rarely” to “almost every day,” where higher scores represent a greater frequency of conversations. Reponses to the two health conversation questions were averaged, and the responses to the four weight conversation questions were averaged resulting in an overall score from 1 to 5 for each subscale (health conversations Cronbach's α = .89; weight conversations Cronbach's α = .83). 2.4 | Statistical analysis All analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics version 25. To establish baseline relationships, we calculated bivariate Pearson correlations amongst the following variables: weight bias internalization, experienced weight stigma, the four parental weight talk measures, parental BMI, and child BMI percentile. To examine the first and second aim of the study, we conducted four linear regression models that assessed experienced weight stigma and weight bias internalization as separate predictors of the four forms of parental 2.3.4 | Weight comments about oneself and others weight talk, while controlling for parental BMI, child BMI percentile, parent and child sex, parent and child age, race, household income, Parental comments about their own weight and others' weight were and education. To address the third aim of the study, the regression measured with two items previously tested in samples of parents of models that were significant at the 0.05 level were further tested for children under the age of 18 years.3,33 Parents responded to the fol- mediation using the PROCESS macro for SPSS.35 We ran three sepa- lowing questions: “Thinking about your child(ren) who you consider rate mediation models to examine whether weight bias internalization to be [underweight/about the right weight/overweight], how often mediated the relationship between experienced weight stigma and do you talk about your own weight, shape, or size with your chil- child‐centred weight conversations, parental comments about oneself, d(ren)?” and “… how often do you make comments about other peo- and parental comments about others. Using the PROCESS macro, we ple's weight, shape, or size with your child(ren)?” Participants conducted a mediation analysis using bootstrap sampling with 5,000 responded using a five‐point Likert scale ranging from “never” to “very replications, which constructs a 95% confidence interval (CI) for the often,” where higher scores indicated a greater frequency of com- indirect effect of the independent variable (experienced weight ments. Similar to previous research using these questions,3 a low fre- stigma) on the dependent variables (the various forms of weight talk) quency of parents in our sample reported using either form of weight via weight bias internalization. The presence of an indirect effect commentary “very often.” To align with prior research practices using was determined if the results of the CI did not contain zero.35 As with PUDNEY ET AL. ---------------------------w• the regression models, we adjusted the mediation models for parental TABLE 3 LEY~•W•®Ht+t·)·t47 'rat- - '~-5 of 11 Correlations between key study variables BMI, child BMI percentile, parent and child sex, parent and child age, 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 race, household income, and education. 1. Weight bias internalization 3 RESULTS | 2. Experienced stigmaa .45*** 3.1 | Relationship between parental weight stigma and weight talk 3. Child‐centred weight conversations .23*** .04 Mean scores on the primary measures are shown inTable 2. The average 4. Child‐centred health conversations .06 5. Comments about self .28*** .20*** .47*** .24*** 6. Comments about others .10* 7. Parent BMI .44*** .37*** .07 8. Child BMI percentile .11* score on the WBIS‐M was 2.91 (SD = 1.59, range 1‐7). Correlations between the main variables are shown in Table 3. Weight bias internalization was correlated with experienced stigma (r(442) = .45, P < .001) and both weight bias internalization and experienced stigma were correlated with parental BMI (r(445) = .44, P < .001; r(442) = .37, P < .001). Parental experiences of weight stigma were not associated with child‐centred weight conversations (β = .02, P = .690), child‐centred health conversations (β = .09, P = .102), or parental weight comments about others (β = .06, P = .248), after adjusting for parental BMI, child BMI percentile, parent and child sex, parent and child age, race, household income, and education. Experienced weight stigma was associated with parental weight comments about oneself (β = .15, P = .003) after adjusting for the control variables. Greater weight bias internalization amongst parents was associ- .10* .05 0.06 .38*** .43*** .12* .05 .17*** .04 .62*** .17*** .05 .15*** .13** .18*** Abbreviation: BMI, body mass index. a Variable is binary. *P < .05. **P < .01. ***P < .001. ated with a greater frequency of child‐centred weight conversations amount of variance in each dependent variable (all P's < .001). The (β = .24, P < .001), parental weight comments about oneself model assessing child‐centred health conversations did not account (β = .29, P < .001), and parental weight comments about others for a significant amount of variance in child‐centred health conversa- (β = .13, P = .017) after adjusting for the control variables. Weight bias tions, so coefficients were not interpreted. Across the three significant internalization was not associated with child‐centred health conversa- mediation models, experienced weight stigma was indirectly associated tions (β = .06, P = .261) after adjusting for the control variables. with greater frequency of child‐centred weight conversations (Bootstrapped CI: 0.07, 0.22), as well as parental weight comments 3.2 talk | Mediational models assessing parental weight about oneself (Bootstrapped CI: 0.10, 0.27), and others (Bootstrapped CI: 0.01, 0.15) through weight bias internalization (Figure 1). Parents' experienced weight stigma was significantly associated with greater As depicted in Table 4, the mediation models assessing child‐centred weight bias internalization in all three models (β = 0.32, P < .001). weight conversations, parental weight comments about oneself, and Additionally, weight bias internalization was associated with an parental weight comments about others explained a significant increased frequency of child‐centred weight conversations (β = .26, P < .001), parental weight comments about oneself (β = .26, P < .001), TABLE 2 and parental weight comments about others (β = .12, P = .034). Primary measures Several consistent relationships emerged between parental weight Range N M SD Min Max Cronbach's Alpha WBIS‐M 445 2.91 1.59 1.00 7.00 .95 conversations (β = .28, P < .001), parental weight comments about Child‐centred weight conversations 442 1.70 0.83 1.00 4.75 .83 themselves (β = .24, P < .001), and parental weight comments about Child‐centred health conversations 441 3.13 1.17 1.00 5.00 .89 centred weight conversations (β = .11, P = .024) and made more fre- Weight comments about oneself 440 2.26 1.01 1.00 4.00 Weight comments about others 439 1.95 0.90 1.00 4.00 talk variables and control variables (see Table 4). Across all three models, child age was positively associated with child‐centred weight others (β = .25, P < .001). Fathers, in this sample, had more child‐ quent comments about others (β = .11, P = .023), relative to mothers. No effect of parent sex emerged for conversations about parent's own weight. Child BMI percentile had a small but significant positive rela- Abbreviation: WBIS‐M, modified weight bias internalization scale. tionship with child‐centred weight conversations (β = .13, P = .005), comments about parent's own body weight (β = .10, P = .026), and comments about others (β = .11, P = .026). --~-w1 LEY~•@•t®iiti•]•r41 i 6 of 11 TABLE 4 PUDNEY ET AL. M f---------------------------- Predictors of parental weight talk Weight conversations R2 df F P 0.16 11, 425 7.28 <.001 B Weight bias internalization SE β t P Bootstrapped 95% CI 0.13 0.03 0.26 4.80 <.001 0.08 0.19 Experienced stigma −0.10 0.09 −0.06 −1.19 .233 −0.27 0.07 Parent BMI −0.01 0.01 −0.07 −1.26 .208 −0.02 0.00 Child BMI %ile 0.00 0.00 0.13 2.80 .005 0.00 0.01 Parent male (ref. female) 0.18 0.08 0.11 2.27 0.024 0.02 0.33 Child male (ref. female) 0.05 0.08 0.03 0.64 0.523 −0.10 0.20 Parent age 0.00 0.01 −0.04 −0.62 .533 −0.02 0.01 Child age 0.05 0.01 0.28 5.03 <.001 0.03 0.07 White (ref. non‐White) −0.16 0.09 −0.08 −1.82 .070 −0.34 0.01 Income −0.05 0.03 −0.08 −1.68 .094 −0.11 0.01 Education −0.06 0.04 −0.08 −1.69 .091 −0.13 0.01 0.08 0.22 Indirect effect of experienced stigma Health conversations .14* 0.04 11, 424 1.68 0.04 .08* .080 Weight bias internalization 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.61 .542 0.10 0.23 Experienced stigma 0.17 0.13 0.07 1.27 .206 −0.06 0.35 Parent BMI 0.00 0.01 −0.01 −0.16 .870 −0.01 0.02 Child BMI %ile 0.00 0.00 0.06 1.16 .246 0.00 0.01 −0.11 0.12 −0.05 −0.94 .348 −0.27 0.34 Parent male (ref. female) Child male (ref. female) 0.14 0.12 0.06 1.24 .216 −0.07 0.28 Parent age 0.01 0.01 0.08 1.20 .230 −0.01 0.02 Child age White (ref. non‐White) Income Education Indirect effect of experienced stigma Comments about oneself 0.19 11, 423 8.90 0.02 0.01 0.10 1.61 .109 0.03 0.07 −0.16 0.14 −0.06 −1.18 .238 −0.48 −0.06 0.01 0.04 0.01 0.25 .802 −0.15 −0.01 −0.01 0.05 −0.01 −0.13 .896 0.02 0.04 0.01 −0.12 0.05 −0.06 0.12 <.001 Weight bias internalization 0.16 0.03 0.26 4.92 <.001 0.10 0.23 Experienced stigma 0.14 0.11 0.07 1.36 .176 −0.06 0.35 Parent BMI 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.10 .920 −0.01 0.02 Child BMI %ile 0.00 0.00 0.10 2.24 .026 0.00 0.01 Parent male (ref. female) 0.16 0.09 0.08 1.68 .094 −0.03 0.34 Child male (ref. female) 0.11 0.09 0.05 1.16 .245 −0.07 0.28 Parent age 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.18 .855 −0.01 0.02 Child age 0.05 0.01 0.24 4.30 <.001 0.03 0.07 White (ref. non‐White) −0.27 0.11 −0.11 −2.55 .011 −0.48 −0.06 Income −0.08 0.04 −0.11 −2.26 .024 −0.15 −0.01 Education −0.03 0.04 −0.04 −0.80 .426 −0.12 0.05 0.10 0.27 Indirect effect of experienced stigma Comments about others .17* 0.09 11, 433 3.62 0.04 .08* <.001 Weight bias internalization 0.07 0.03 0.12 2.11 .036 0.00 0.13 Experienced stigma 0.04 0.10 0.02 0.43 .671 −0.15 0.24 −0.01 0.01 −0.04 −0.64 .523 −0.02 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.11 2.23 .026 0.00 0.01 Parent BMI Child BMI %ile (Continues) PUDNEY ET AL. ---------------------------w• TABLE 4 LEY~•W•®Ht+t·)·t47 'rat----'~-7 of 11 (Continued) R2 df F P B SE β t P Bootstrapped 95% CI Parent male (ref. female) 0.21 0.09 0.11 2.29 .023 0.03 0.38 Child male (ref. female) 0.11 0.09 0.06 1.22 .222 −0.06 0.28 −0.01 0.01 −0.07 −1.15 .252 −0.02 0.01 Parent age Child age 0.05 0.01 0.25 4.21 <.001 0.02 0.07 White (ref. non‐White) −0.16 0.10 −0.07 −1.52 .128 −0.36 0.05 Income −0.03 0.03 −0.04 −0.74 .461 −0.09 0.04 0.01 0.04 0.02 0.31 .761 −0.07 0.09 0.01 0.15 Education Indirect effect of experienced stigma .07* 0.04 .04* Note. All models adjust for BMI, child BMI percentile, parent and child sex, parent and child age, race, income, and education. P < .001 is displayed where P could not be exactly expressed within three decimal points. B = unstandardized regression coefficient, β = standardized regression coefficient. The standard errors for each indirect effect are bootstrapped. Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; SE, standard error. *Significance is based on the bootstrapped CI not containing zero. B= 1.04, fi=.32, P<.001 Parent's Weight Bias Internalization B= 0.13,P = .26, P< .001 Parent's Child-Centered Experienced Weight Conversations Stigma c': B = -0.l0,P= -,06, P= ·229 ~ - - - - ~ (Bootstrapped CI for indirect effect: 0.08, 0.22) ~ - - - - ~ Parent's Parent's B= 1.04, P=.32, P<.001 Weight Bias Internalization B=0.17, p=.26, P<.001 Parent's Weight Experienced Comments Stigma c': B = 0.14, P= .07, P = .194 About Oneself ~ - - - - ~ (Bootstrapped CI for indirect effect: 0. 10, 0.27) ~ - - - - ~ FIGURE 1 4 | B= 1.04,fi=.32,P<.001 Weight Bias Internalization B = 0.07,P= .12, P= .034 Parent's Weight Experienced Comments 02 695 Stigma c': B = 0.04, P= - ,P= · About Other.; ~----~(Bootstrapped CI for indirect effect: 0.01 , 0.1 5 ) ~ - - - - ~ Mediator models for parental weight talk DISCUSSION align with some previous research documenting differing outcomes when comparing external stigma with internal, self‐stigma, such as This study is the first to examine the role of parental weight stigma with exercise behaviours and self‐esteem.25,26 In order to gain a better (experienced weight stigma and internalized weight bias) in parental understanding of how experienced and internalized weight stigma comments and communication about weight. Findings suggest that may differ in their implications for weight talk, it will be informative parents' experienced weight stigma was indirectly associated with for future research to examine parental perceptions of whether their child‐centred weight conversations and parental weight comments experiences of weight stigma and/or their internalization of stigma via parents' internalized weight bias. are related to their reasons for engaging in weight talk and to assess These findings show that experienced weight stigma was related whether certain aspects of parental weight stigma, such as the time to greater weight bias internalization amongst parents, and that period of when stigmatization occurred, the source of their stigmatiz- greater internalization of weight bias in parents was in turn signifi- ing experiences, and the extent of distress and internalization in cantly associated with a higher frequency of child‐centred weight con- response to different types of weight stigma, play a role in their moti- versations and parental weight comments about oneself and others. vations for engaging in or avoiding weight talk with their child. Ideally, Accounting for weight bias internalization in the models eliminated longitudinal research would be valuable to identify the relationships the effect of experienced stigma, suggesting that experienced weight between experienced stigma, internalized weight bias, and engage- stigma influences child‐centred weight conversations and parental ment in weight talk over time. Of note, accounting for parental weight comments indirectly via weight bias internalization. These findings stigma did not explain a significant amount of the variance in child‐ --~-w1 LEY~•@•t®iiti•]•r41 i 8 of 11 PUDNEY ET AL. M f---------------------------- centred health conversations, suggesting that weight stigma may be greater frequency of weight‐focused conversations and comments as particularly important in the context of child‐centred conversations children get older. Moreover, the association between child age and that focus on body weight rather than health. While pediatric pro- parental weight talk variables was equally as strong as weight bias viders may want to encourage all parents to promote healthy lifestyle internalization for child‐centred weight conversations and for behaviours in their children, parents who internalize weight bias may weight‐focused comments that parents made about themselves. Addi- particularly benefit from encouragement to focus on their child's tionally, the association between child age and parental weight com- health behaviours rather than his/her weight, as our findings suggest ments about others was twice as strong as the relationship between that these parents may be especially likely to engage in weight‐ parent weight comments about others and weight bias internalization. focused comments and conversations with their child. Most of the existing research on parental weight talk has focused There could be several reasons why greater internalized weight on adolescents, rather than children of all ages, but a recent study of bias is related to parental engagement in conversations with their child 2‐ to 17‐year‐old children found child‐centred weight conversations about their child's weight and the practice of making weight com- to be more frequent with older children.38 For children going through ments about themselves or others. Parents who internalize weight puberty, weight conversations may occur because parents are more bias may have heightened concerns that their child will be teased aware of the change in their child's appearance and/or because chil- about weight and thus be more likely to intervene by talking to their dren may make more comments about their own body during this child about his/her weight, eating behaviours, and/or physical activity developmental period, sparking conversations. Therefore, future in order to lose weight and avoid potential teasing. Additionally, par- research should examine patterns and potential motivations for ents who internalize weight bias may be more likely than those who weight talk at different stages in child development and whether do not internalize to be fixated on their own (or others') weight, and parental weight talk affects children and adolescents differently in turn be more likely to make weight‐based comments. As it is unclear depending on their age. whether parents engage in these behaviours intending to influence In addition, there was a positive relationship between child BMI their child or whether they are not cognizant of their intentions, future percentile and frequency of child‐centred weight conversations, research should examine parental awareness and intentions of their parental weight comments about oneself, and parental weight com- child‐centred communication and potential underlying mechanisms ments about others. Even though child BMI was not as strong of a pre- that can help clarify the ways in that experienced weight stigma versus dictor as child age, our findings do suggest that frequency of child‐ internalized weight bias are related to parental weight talk. Further- centred weight conversations increase with child BMI. These findings more, it will be informative to examine the role of other parental char- align with recent evidence documenting a higher frequency of child‐ acteristics that might be at play in parental weight talk, such as centred weight conversations in children with a higher BMI,6,39 disordered eating behaviours amongst parents and/or their child. highlighting the importance of ensuring that children with higher body In line with a recent policy statement from the American Academy weight (who are particularly vulnerable to weight stigma36) are not of Pediatrics (AAP) that aims to raise awareness amongst pediatric further stigmatized in parent‐child conversations about weight. A providers about weight stigma in children and adolescents who may recent study found that some adolescents with higher body weight be vulnerable to teasing and bullying because of their weight,36 our felt ashamed and embarrassed by parental weight talk and preferred findings suggest that it may be additionally useful for pediatric pro- that parents use neutral terminology when discussing their weight.40 viders to raise awareness about the potential implications of weight Providers can encourage parents to ask their children about words stigma in parents, which could contribute to the nature and frequency or phrases that they would like their parents to avoid. Given the cur- of weight‐focused communication with their children. Future research rent findings demonstrating associations between a child's body size should also explore strategies to prevent and reduce weight bias inter- and frequency of parental weight comments directed toward oneself, nalization in parents. future research should clarify the effect of a child's weight on different It is important to note that mean WBIS‐M score in this sample types of parental weight talk. (M = 2.91) was lower than similar general population samples, which It is noteworthy that the present findings showed that fathers typically range from 3.27 to 3.95.18,19,21,24 Despite these lower levels were more likely than mothers to engage in weight conversations of internalized weight bias in our sample, we found significant positive and make comments about others, but there were no differences in relationships between internalized weight bias and frequency of child‐ mothers' versus fathers' weight comments about themselves. This centred weight conversations and parental weight comments. Thus, it finding is contrary to other studies that have documented similar rates will be informative to examine patterns of weight talk amongst par- of weight talk between mothers and fathers6 or higher rates of weight ents with higher levels of internalized weight bias as well as parents talk from mothers compared with fathers.41,42 While it is beyond the engaged in weight loss treatment, who may have heightened vulnera- scope of our study to identify reasons for differences in weight talk bility to weight stigma.37 between fathers and mothers, one potential explanation for this find- Our study findings additionally identified consistent relationships ing could be different perspectives of parents and children. Previous between several control variables and parental weight talk variables. studies typically assess parental weight talk from the perspective of Specifically, we found child age to be a strong positive predictor in the child, as opposed to the parent,5 but studies that have compared all three significant models of parental weight talk, suggesting a reports of parents with those of their children have pointed to PUDNEY ET AL. --------------------------w• LEY~•W•®Ht+t·)·t47 'rat- - '~-9 of 11 potential differences in frequency of weight talk.2,3 Furthermore, it Despite these limitations, our study offers several key strengths. This may be that parental sex affects not just the frequency of weight talk, is the first study to assess the extent to that parental experiences of but the topics of these conversations and the sex of the child they weight stigma and weight bias internalization are associated with parental engage with. The limited evidence in this area has found that mothers weight talk. Additionally, the use of multiple measures of parental weight tend to engage in food‐related weight talk, while fathers tend to com- talk allowed for an informative description of different components of ment on specific body parts41; mothers engage in more weight talk parental weight communication. Finally, the inclusion of parents with chil- with their daughters, and fathers with their sons6; and appearance dren ages 2 to 17 years in our sample improves upon previous studies on 43 teasing by fathers can be particularly damaging to their daughters. parental weight talk that typically target limited age groups of children. Therefore, it will be informative for future work to assess weight talk from the perspective of both parents and their child(ren) to better 4.2 | Conclusion understand how parental sex plays a role in weight‐focused communication. Survey measures that can capture potential distinctions Our findings offer novel insights into the unexplored relationship between mothers and fathers will be key in these efforts to accurately between weight stigma and parental weight talk. This study suggests understand these family dynamics, which can in turn inform pediatric that parents' internalized weight bias mediates the relationship providers. between their experienced weight stigma and the frequency of child‐ centred weight conversations and parental weight comments about oneself and others. Given that these forms of parental weight talk 4.1 | Limitations can have harmful effects on child health,3,4 this study offers important insights about parental factors that may increase these behaviours. There are several limitations of this study. First, the cross‐sectional The AAP recommends that pediatricians be aware of weight stigma nature of this study precludes causal inferences between parental and its adverse implications for children and adolescents36 and to edu- weight stigma and their engagement in weight‐based comments cate parents about the best practices regarding ways to communicate and/or conversations with their child. It will be informative for future about weight‐related health with children, such as discouraging dieting research to assess this relationship longitudinally to help determine and focusing on healthy eating and physical activity instead of whether parental weight stigma influences parental weight talk over weight.49 Our findings suggest that it may be additionally useful for time and whether education about weight talk from pediatric providers pediatric providers to be aware of internalized weight bias amongst affects the incidence of parental weight talk. Second, MTurk is not a parents and its potential role in parental weight talk. national panel, and the sample of parents was predominately White and highly educated, limiting generalizability and indicating the need for future research to examine more ethnically and economically diverse samples. Third, measures were self‐reported, including the ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors are grateful to Alison Manchester for her assistance in managing data collection for this study. height and weight of both parents and their children. Although objective measures of height and weight are ideal, studies show that online self‐reported heights and weights are a valid alternative to measured CON F L I C T S OF IN TE RE S T height and weight.44,45 The survey questions we used to assess paren- The other authors have indicated they have no financial relationships tal comments and conversations about weight have been used in stud- relevant to this article to disclose. ies with similar populations, but future studies should aim to develop more comprehensive, validated tools that measure different types of AUTHOR CONT R IBUT IONS weight talk from the perspectives of both children and parents. For R.M.P. conceptualized the study. E.V.P. and M.S.H. carried out data example, parents might not be fully aware of the extent that they talk analyses, and E.V.P. wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. All about weight, and their self‐report could be biased. In examining expe- authors revised the manuscript, approved the final manuscript as sub- rienced stigma, we did not assess the frequency, recency, or distress mitted, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. arising from the stigmatizing experiences. While previous longitudinal research suggests that dichotomous questions (ie, yes/no questions) ORCID about the presence versus absence of weight labelling or stigmatizing Ellen V. Pudney experiences can inform long‐term associations with maladaptive eating Mary S. Himmelstein behaviours and poor weight‐related health,46-48 it will be important for Rebecca M. Puhl https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7197-5783 https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3173-1901 https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2340-2486 future research to include more comprehensive measures of stigmatizing experiences (ie, distress, recency, and frequency) to better under- RE FE RE NC ES stand the nature and extent of parental experiences with weight stigma. Finally, because of the few measures available in the literature to examine internalized weight bias, future studies should continue to assess the long‐term stability and predictive validity of the WBIS‐M. 1. Sinton MM, Goldschmidt AB, Aspen V, et al. Psychosocial correlates of shape and weight concerns in overweight pre‐adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41(1):67‐75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964‐ 011‐9686‐y --~~w1 LEv-1•W•®iiits·)·141 ii?f---------------------------10 of 11 2. Neumark‐Sztainer D, Bauer KW, Friend S, Hannan PJ, Story M, Berge JM. Family weight talk and dieting: how much do they matter for body dissatisfaction and disordered eating behaviors in adolescent girls? J Adolesc Health. 2010;47(3):270‐276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jadohealth.2010.02.001 3. Bauer KW, Bucchianeri MM, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Mother‐reported parental weight talk and adolescent girls' emotional health, weight control attempts, and disordered eating behaviors. J Eat Disord. 2013;1(1):2‐9. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050‐2974‐1‐45 4. Berge JM, MacLehose RF, Loth KA, Eisenberg ME, Bucchianeri MM, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Parent conversations about healthful eating and weight associations with adolescent disordered eating behaviors. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(8):746‐753. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapedia trics.2013.78 5. Gillison FB, Lorenc AB, Sleddens EFC, Williams SL, Atkinson L. Can it be harmful for parents to talk to their child about their weight? A meta‐analysis. Prev Med (Baltim). 2016;93:135‐146. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.10.010 6. Berge JM, MacLehose RF, Loth KA, Eisenberg ME, Fulkerson JA, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Parent‐adolescent conversations about eating, physical activity and weight: prevalence across sociodemographic characteristics and associations with adolescent weight and weight‐related behaviors. J Behav Med. 2014;38(1):122‐135. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10865‐014‐9584‐3 7. Puhl RM, Himmelstein MS. A word to the wise: adolescent reactions to parental communication about weight. Child Obes. 2018;14(5): 291‐301. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2018.0047 8. Lumeng JC, Forrest P, Appugliese DP, Kaciroti N, Corwyn RF, Bradley RH. Weight status as a predictor of being bullied in third through sixth grades. Pediatrics. 2010;125(6):e1301‐e1307. https://doi.org/ 10.1542/peds.2009‐0774 9. Puhl RM, Latner JD, O'brien K, Luedicke J, Forhan M, Danielsdottir S. Cross‐national perspectives about weight‐based bullying in youth: nature, extent and remedies. Pediatr Obes. 2015;11(4):241‐250. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12051. 10. Puhl RM, Luedicke J, Heuer CA. Weight‐based victimization toward overweight adolescents: observations and reactions of peers. J Sch Health. 2011;81(11):696‐703. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/ 10.1111/j.1746‐1561.2011.00646.x. Accessed September 7, 2018. 11. Puhl RM, Peterson JL, Luedicke J. Weight‐based victimization: bullying experiences of weight loss treatment–seeking youth. Pediatrics. 2013;131(1):1‐9. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012‐1106 12. Kluck AS. Family influence on disordered eating: the role of body image dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2010;7(1):8‐14. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.009 13. Berge JM, Trofholz A, Fong S, Blue L, Neumark‐Sztainer D. A qualitative analysis of parents' perceptions of weight talk and weight teasing in the home environments of diverse low‐income children. Body Image. 2015;15:8‐15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.006 14. Himmelstein MS, Puhl RM, Quinn DM. Intersectionality: an understudied framework for addressing weight stigma. Am J Prev Med. 2017;53(4):421‐431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2017. 04.003 15. Puhl RM, Latner JD, O'Brien K, Luedicke J, Danielsdottir S, Forhan M. A multinational examination of weight bias: predictors of anti‐fat attitudes across four countries. Int J Obes (Lond). 2015;39(7):1166‐1173. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2015.32 16. Puhl RM, Suh Y. Health consequences of weight stigma: implications for obesity prevention and treatment. Curr Obes Rep. 2015;4(2): 182‐190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13679‐015‐0153‐z PUDNEY ET AL. 17. Vartanian LR, Porter AM. Weight stigma and eating behavior: a review of the literature. Appetite. 2016;102:3‐14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. appet.2016.01.034 18. Puhl RM, Himmelstein MS, Quinn DM. Internalizing weight stigma: prevalence and sociodemographic considerations in US adults. Obesity. 2018;26(1):167‐175. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22029 19. Durso LE, Latner JD. Understanding self‐directed stigma: development of the weight bias internalization scale. Obesity. 2008;16(SUPPL. 2): S80‐S86. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.448 20. Lee MS, Dedrick RF. Weight bias internalization scale: psychometric properties using alternative weight status classification approaches. Body Image. 2016;17:25‐29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016. 01.008 21. Pearl RL, Puhl RM. Measuring internalized weight attitudes across body weight categories: validation of the modified weight Bias internalization scale. Body Image. 2014;11(1):89‐92. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.09.005 22. O'Brien KS, Latner JD, Puhl RM, et al. The relationship between weight stigma and eating behavior is explained by weight bias internalization and psychological distress. Appetite. 2016;102:70‐76. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.032 23. Pearl RL, Wadden TA, Hopkins CM, et al. Association between weight bias internalization and metabolic syndrome among treatment‐seeking individuals with obesity. Obesity. 2017;25(2):317‐322. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/oby.21716 24. Latner JD, Barile JP, Durso LE, O'Brien KS. Weight and health‐related quality of life: the moderating role of weight discrimination and internalized weight bias. Eat Behav. 2014;15(4):586‐590. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.08.014 25. Pearl RL, Puhl RM. The distinct effects of internalizing weight bias: an experimental study. Body Image. 2016;17:38‐42. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.002 26. Pearl RL, Puhl RM, Dovidio JF. Differential effects of weight bias experiences and internalization on exercise among women with overweight and obesity. J Health Psychol. 2015;20(12):1626‐1632. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/1359105313520338 27. Hunger JM, Major B. Weight stigma mediates the association between BMI and self‐reported health. Health Psychol. 2015;34(2):172‐175. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000106 28. Statistics NC for H. Table 53. Selected Health Conditions and Risk Factors, by Age: United States, Selected Years 1988–1994 through 2015– 2016; 2017. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/hus/contents2017.htm#053. Accessed February 10, 2019. 29. Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk: understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(3):184‐188. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721414531598 30. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon's Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high‐quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(1):3‐5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980 31. Centers for Disease Control. Defining overweight and obesity: Overweight and obesity. Available at http://www.cdc.gov/obesity/adult/ defining.html. Published 2012. Accessed September 16, 2016. 32. Lytle LA, Birnbaum A, Boutelle K, Murray DM. Wellness and risk communication from parent to teen: the “parental energy index”. Health Educ. 1999;99(5):207‐214. 33. Neumark‐Sztainer D, Haines J, Robinson‐O'Brien R, et al. “Ready. Set. ACTION!” A theater‐based obesity prevention program for children: a feasibility study. Health Educ Res. 2009;24(3):407‐420. https://doi. org/10.1093/her/cyn036 PUDNEY ET AL. -------------------------WI 34. Puhl RM, Heuer CA, Sarda V. Framing messages about weight discrimination: impact on public support for legislation. Int J Obes (Lond). 2011;35(6):863‐872. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2010.194 35. Hayes AF. PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. White Pap 2012; 1–39. Available at http://www.afhayes.com/public/ process2012.pdf LEY~•@•®iiiti•]•I41; Ml-- "~ -11 of 11 43. Keery H, Boutelle K, Van Den Berg P, Thompson JK. The impact of appearance‐related teasing by family members. J Adolesc Health. 2005;37(2):120‐127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.08. 015 44. Ekström S, Kull I, Nilsson S, Bergström A. Web‐based self‐reported height, weight, and body mass index among Swedish adolescents: a validation study. J Med Internet Res. 2015;17(3). 36. Pont SJ, Puhl RM, Cook SR, Slusser W. Stigma experienced by children and adolescents with obesity. Pediatrics. 2017;140(6):1‐11. https://doi. org/10.1542/peds.2017‐3034 45. Pursey K, Burrows TL, Stanwell P, Collins CE. How accurate is web‐ based self‐reported height, weight, and body mass index in young adults. J Med Internet Res. 2014;16(1). 37. Carels RA, Wott CB, Young KM, Gumble A, Koball AM, Oehlhof MW. Implicit, explicit, and internalized weight bias and psychosocial maladjustment among treatment‐seeking adults. Eat Behav. 2010;11(3): 180‐185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.03.002. 46. Hunger JM, Tomiyama AJ. Weight labeling and disordered eating among adolescent girls: longitudinal evidence from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute growth and health study. J Adolesc Health. 2018;63(3):360‐362. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JADOHEALTH.2017. 12.016. 38. Winkler MR, Berge JM, Larson N, Loth KA, Wall M, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Parent‐child health‐ and weight‐focused conversations: who is saying what and to whom? Appetite. 2018;126(March):114‐120. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.03.023 39. Eisenberg ME, Berge JM, Fulkerson JA, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Weight comments by family and significant others in young adulthood. Body Image. 2011;8(1):12‐19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010. 11.002 40. Puhl RM, Himmelstein MS, Armstrong S, Kingsford E. Adolescent preferences and reactions to language about body weight. Int J Obes (Lond). 2017;41(7):1062‐1065. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2017.55 41. Berge JM, Hanson‐Bradley parents or siblings engage with children and what does Body Image. 2016;18:27‐33. 04.008 C, Tate A, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Do in more negative weight‐based talk it sound like? A mixed‐methods study. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016. 42. Taylor CB, Bryson S, Celio Doyle AA, et al. The adverse effect of negative comments about weight and shape from family and siblings on women at high risk for eating disorders. Pediatrics. 2006;118(2): 731‐738. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005‐1806 47. Puhl RM, Wall M, Chen C, Bryn Austin S, Eisenberg ME, Neumark‐ Sztainer D. Experiences of weight teasing in adolescence and weight‐ related outcomes in adulthood: a 15‐year longitudinal study. Prev Med (Baltim). 2017;100:173‐179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed. 2017.04.023 48. Hunger JM, Tomiyama AJ. Weight labeling and obesity: a 10‐year longitudinal study of girls aged 10‐19. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(6): 579‐580. 49. Golden NH, Schneider M, Wood C. Preventing obesity and eating disorders in adolescents. Pediatrics. 2016;138(3):e1‐e10. https://doi.org/ 10.1542/peds.2016‐1649 How to cite this article: Pudney EV, Himmelstein MS, Puhl RM. The role of weight stigma in parental weight talk. Pediatric Obesity. 2019;e12534. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12534
Photograph of Author